Harvard Business School’s Working Knowledge recently published a working paper on how politicians successfully dodge questions with implications for all of us who face or ask questions.
The working paper, “Conversational Blindness: Answering the Wrong Question the Right Way” was written by Todd Rogers and Michael Norton and published in the Oct 20, 2008 issue of HBS Working Knowledge.
This research on politicians is highly relevant for primary researchers. The HBS study used audiotapes of politicians providing a healthcare answer to four questions; one on education, one on healthcare, a similar one on the drug problem in the US, and a dissimilar one on the war on terror.
The study found that almost half the listeners to the question on the drug problem believed that the politician had answered the question appropriately while most participants who heard the question on the war on terror recognized that the healthcare answer did not fit.
Even more interesting was the result of the second phase of the study in which the politician hemmed and hawed his way through the answer. His apparent uncertainty caused his listeners to doubt his appropriate answer and prefer a polished, confident healthcare answer to the drug problem question over the unpolished healthcare answer to the healthcare question.
The HBS researchers believe that this phenomenon occurs because listeners are limited in their capacity to absorb information coming at them, plus people correlate a confident manner with accurate answers.
“Conversational blindness occurs in part because real-world conversations occur as a continuous ebb and flow, leaving little time for people to reflect on how each statement links to each previous statement. Indeed, people’s inability to notice not just subtle changes to their environment but even obvious changes has been traced to the inherent limitations of attention (Neisser, 1979; Simons & Chabris 1999). Thus the recall that we observe in our experiments is, if anything, an overestimate of people’s ability to detect dodges, since we presented participants with a forced-choice four-option question—giving them a 25% chance at the right answer—a situation that does not arise naturally in the world. Accordingly, decreasing conversational blindness in everyday interactions may be no easy task.”
There are several implications of this study for primary research:
If you are a primary researcher who does not want to answer a question on the sponsor of the study, provide an answer that is very close to the direct answer to the question and that addresses common, but unexpressed, concerns. For example, many sources are not allowed to speak with reporters and must refer all calls to PR. If I answer the “Who is the sponsor?” question with “We’re just trying to get background information. This is not for publication or attribution,” I’ve answered the hidden question of “Will I get into trouble if I speak with this person?”
Speak with confidence when answering any questions.
Listen very carefully for dodges when speaking with a source. While the beauty of primary research is the ability for conversations to flow in important directions, we still need to get answers to certain questions and may have to circle around again to a question if it is dodged. Do not let the confidence tone of the source deflect your question.
To read the complete working paper, go to http://hbswk.hbs.edu/item/6050.html.
References cited in the quotation.
Neisser, U. (1999). The control of information pickup in selective lookup. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its Development: A Tribute to Eleanor A. Gibson (pp. 201-219) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Simons, D.J. and Chabris, C.F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059-1074.
Recent Comments